Professor Jens Brockmeier
In June, in acknowledgment of his lifetime achievement, Professor Jens Brockmeier of the Department of Psychology, Health and Gender was awarded the Ernst E. Boesch Prize 2021, which recognizes outstanding scholarly achievement in the field of cultural psychology. The prize is awarded to researchers and scholars whose work has made a significant and enduring impact. The award ceremony took place in Ascona, Switzerland, in September 2021.
Cultural psychology is a branch of psychology that looks at the ways in which an individuals culture and society affect their mind and behavior. Traditional psychology looks at what is universal or what all people have in common at any rate, what you can measure, says Brockmeier. Cultural psychologists look at what is specific, local and unique. The approach views human beings as members of a larger sociocultural world as people who are always together with others, living in cultural communities that differ vastly across the planet. You cannot just compare love, for example, in Boston or Ghana to love in Tokyo or Paris.
The field has what Brockmeier describes as an outsider status, due to a tension that exists between the universalist approach to human nature proposed by traditional psychology and the cultural-psychological interest in particularities and singularities. Cultural psychologists make use of different methodologies, such as longform narrative interviews, which are less grounded in statistics or other natural-science methods and more grounded in the experience of life. Subjects, for example, are asked for their own views and definitions of emotions or memories; researchers then place any responses in the context of their communities. Its rare that the kind of investigation done by cultural psychologists and cultural anthropologists is recognized, explains Brockmeier. Therefore, I was very happy to be given this award. He is particularly grateful to his 91勛圖厙colleagues for being open and encouraging of the kind of research that exists at the margins of mainstream psychology.
Brockmeiers interdisciplinary academic background extends far beyond psychology; he holds a PhD in philosophy from Freie Universit瓣t Berlin. I was also very interested in literature, history and art, he says. I came to psychology because I wanted to know how we use literature, artworks and music to understand the world and ourselves. Why do we invent imaginary universes all the time? The discipline allowed Brockmeier to explore a wide variety of topics that he would otherwise not be able to explore. Rather than be pigeonholed in a single cultural field such as art, philosophy or literature, he chose to explore how all his passions were connected. Cultural psychology allows him to explore psychological topics, such as consciousness or identity, while also acknowledging the ways in which people relate to each other in different cultures.
When teaching the subject as in AUPs psychology major, Brockmeier makes regular use of literature, film and other cultural sources to examine different cultural traditions. A prominent example comes from a long-term research project he conducted in Canada with fellow 91勛圖厙professor Maria Medved. The study took a cultural anthropological view of the increased rate of heart conditions among First Nations populations. In class, Brockmeier asks students to consider the ways in which Western medical models are entrenched in certain narratives, such as that of colonialism, which may be alienating to indigenous populations. Cree people define illness differently, he explains. You end up exploring political issues even though you may have intended only to research heart disease. To illustrate this point, Brockmeier draws on First Nations literature and screens Atanarjuat:狼he Fast Runner, a 2001 film set in pre-colonization Canada and made by indigenous filmmakers. He then asks students to reflect on what is different about First Nations peoples own narratives of their cultural traditions.
Brockmeiers current research focuses on understanding time and temporality, not only in the context of measuring and perceiving time but also in the context of how individuals interpret their own lifetimes. How do we see ourselves in the complex, layered system of past, present and future? he asks. We all continuously negotiate this synthesis, but we do it within very different cultural traditions. Brockmeier works with research subjects who operate outside of the traditional narratives of time; he notes increasingly that traditional stories of linear individual time are starting to dissolve. Young people, like our 91勛圖厙students, have such an enormously open horizon, he explains. They combine what I call different islands of temporality circular or elliptical movements that are more fragmented and less part of a single narrative. Brockmeier aims to uncover new perceptions of how a lifetime is defined, exploring how changing cultural models of time shape individual temporalities. He explains, for example, how there are languages and cultures that conceive of time as something that moves backwards. For me, such ideas are even more emphasized because of the lifetime achievement award, he says. Its an odd feeling to have people look back over your life while you are still living it forward!